¿Qué hacen los humanos ante una amenaza colectiva? | 20 ABR 20

Pandemias y el gran desajuste evolutivo

Las personas se asocian y buscan contacto social cuando están expuestas a una amenaza. Esta tendencia, que podría haber sido adaptativa en nuestro pasado evolutivo, es nuestro problema más serio ahora
Autor/a: G. Dezecache, C.Frith y O.Deroy Fuente: Cell Press http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.04.010  Pandemics and the great evolutionary mismatch
INDICE:  1. Texto principal | 2. Referencias bibliográficas
Referencias bibliográficas
1. Dezecache, G. (2015). Human collective reactions to threat. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 6, 209–219.
2. Drury, J. (2018). The role of social identity processes in mass emergency behaviour: An integrative review. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 29, 38–81.
3. Mawson, A.R. (2005). Understanding mass panic and other collective responses to threat and disaster. Psychiatry Interpers. Biol. Process. 68, 95–113.
4. Mawson, A.R. (2017). Mass panic and social attachment: The dynamics of human behavior (Routledge).
5. Morrison, I. (2016). Keep calm and cuddle on: social touch as a stress buffer. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2, 344–362.
6. El Zein, M., Bahrami, B., and Hertwig, R. (2019). Shared responsibility in collective decisions. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 554–559.
7. Koomen, R. (2020). 7 things to know about human psychology when dealing with the corona virus. Medium. Available at: https://medium.com/@rebecca.koomen/7-things-toknow-about-human-psychology-when-dealing-with-the-corona-virus-f011bcb52b9e.
8. Rand, D.G. (2016). Cooperation, Fast and Slow: Meta-Analytic Evidence for a Theory of Social Heuristics and Self-Interested Deliberation. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1192–1206.
9. Rand, D.G., Greene, J.D., and Nowak, M.A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature 489, 427–430.
10. Dezecache, G., Martin, J.-R., Tessier, C., Safra, L., Pitron, V., Nuss, P., and Grèzes, J. (In preparation). Determinants of supportive behavior during a mass shooting.
11. Eisenberger, N.I. (2013). An empirical review of the neural underpinnings of receiving and giving social support: implications for health. Psychosom. Med. 75, 545.
12. Dezecache, G., Grèzes, J., and Dahl, C.D. (2017). The nature and distribution of affiliative behaviour during exposure to mild threat. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4, 170265.
13. Sorokowska, A., Sorokowski, P., Hilpert, P., Cantarero, K., Frackowiak, T., Ahmadi, K., Alghraibeh, A.M., Aryeetey, R., Bertoni, A., Bettache, K., et al. (2017). Preferred Interpersonal Distances: A Global Comparison. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 48, 577–592.
14. Dunbar, R.I.M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 260–268.
15. Dunbar, R.I.M. (2012). Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 1837–1846.
16. Beckes, L., and Coan, J.A. (2011). Social baseline theory: The role of social proximity in emotion and economy of action. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 5, 976–988.
17. Tomova, L., Wang, K., Thompson, T., Matthews, G., Takahashi, A., Tye, K., and Saxe, R. (2020). The need to connect: Acute social isolation causes neural craving responses similar to hunger. bioRxiv, 2020.03.25.006643.
18. Mercier, H. (2020). Not born yesterday: The science of who we trust and what we believe (Princeton University Press).
19. Mercier, H., and Sperber, D. (2011). Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 34, 57–74.
20. Gallotti, M., and Frith, C.D. (2013). Social cognition in the we-mode. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 160–165.
21. Levy, M.R., and Tasoff, J. (2017). Exponential-growth bias and overconfidence. J. Econ. Psychol. 58, 1–14.
22. Stephens, N.M., Hamedani, M.G., Markus, H.R., Bergsieker, H.B., and Eloul, L. (2009). Why did they “choose” to stay? Perspectives of Hurricane Katrina observers and survivors. Psychol. Sci. 20, 878–886.
23. Tybur, J.M., and Lieberman, D. (2016). Human pathogen avoidance adaptations. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7, 6–11.
24. Neuberg, S.L., Kenrick, D.T., and Schaller, M. (2011). Human threat management systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1042–1051.
25. Rozin, P. (1986). One-trial acquired likes and dislikes in humans: Disgust as a US, food predominance, and negative learning predominance. Learn. Motiv. 17, 180–189.
26. Deroy, O. (2015). Eat insects for fun, not to help the environment. Nature 521, 395–395.
27. Rozin, P., and Haidt, J. (2013). The domains of disgust and their origins: Contrasting biological and cultural evolutionary accounts. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 367–368. 10
28. Brooks, S.K., Webster, R.K., Smith, L.E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., and Rubin, G.J. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. The Lancet.
 

Comentarios

Usted debe ingresar al sitio con su cuenta de usuario IntraMed para ver los comentarios de sus colegas o para expresar su opinión. Si ya tiene una cuenta IntraMed o desea registrase, ingrese aquí

Contenidos relacionados
Los editores le recomiendan continuar con las siguientes lecturas: